Research Article | OPEN ACCESS
From SNARC Effect to SELF-SNARC Effect: Evidence from Chinese Character Numbers and Arabic Numbers
1, 2Zhao Xiaojun, 1Shi Changxiu, 2Gan Shuoqiu and 1Hu Chaoyi
1School of Education, Anqing Normal College, Anqing 246133, P.R. China
2School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi
Research Journal of Applied Sciences, Engineering and Technology 2014 6:1107-1112
Received: January 15, 2013 | Accepted: February 07, 2013 | Published: February 15, 2014
Abstract
The presence of the SNARC effect and the SELF-SNARC effect in the processing of various numeric types and how they evolved through representation guidance were investigated through two experiments. The study identified the typical SNARC effect in various numeric types and examined the reversal of the SELF-SNARC effect based on different numeric types and the hypnotic self representation mode. From the perspective of social cognitive semantic conflict and social cognitive reaction conflict, the findings provide a new interpretation of the SELF- SNARC effect.
Keywords:
SELF-SNARC effect, SNARC effect, Social cognitive conflict,
References
-
Brysbaert, M., 1995. Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 124: 434-452.
CrossRef
-
Dehaene, S., S. Bossini and P. Giraux, 1993. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 122: 371-396.
CrossRef
-
Dodd, M.D., 2011. Negative numbers eliminate, but do not reverse, the attentional SNARC effect. Psychol. Res., 75: 2-9.
CrossRef PMid:20379741
-
Fias, W., M. Brysbaert, F. Geypens and G. Ydewalle, 1996. The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: evidence from the SNARC effect. Math. Cognition, 2: 95-110.
CrossRef
-
Fischer, M.H. and J. Rottmann, 2005. Do negative numbers have a place on the mental number line? Psychol. Sci., 47(1): 22-32.
-
Gevers, W., T. Verguts and B. Reynvoet, 2006. Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Human, 32(1): 32-44.
CrossRef PMid:16478324
-
Ito, Y. and T. Hatta, 2004. Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Mem. Cognition, 32(4): 662-673.
CrossRef PMid:15478760
-
Keus, I.M. and W. Schwarz, 2005. Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin. Mem. Cognition, 33(4): 681-695.
CrossRef PMid:16248333
-
Müller, D. and W. Schwarz, 2007a. Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Mem. Cognition, 35(5): 1151-1161.
CrossRef PMid:17910196
-
Müller, D. and W. Schwarz, 2007b. Exploring the mental number line: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Psychol. Res., 71: 598-613.
CrossRef PMid:16786352
-
Nuerk, H., U. Weger and K. Willmes, 2001. Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in the different bins. Cognition, 82(1): 825-833.
CrossRef
-
Pan, Y., D.L. Shen and J. Wang, 2009. The spatial numerical association of response codes effect of Chinese character number processing in different attention referential cues. Stud. Psychol. Behav., 7(1): 21-26.
-
Previtali, P., M.D. Hevia and L. Girelli, 2010. Placing order in space: The SNARC effect in serial learning. Exp. Brain Res., 201: 599-605.
CrossRef PMid:19888566
-
Rugani, R., G. Vallortigara, B. Vallini and L. Regolin, 2011. Asymmetrical number-space mapping in the avian brain. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem., 95: 231-238.
CrossRef PMid:21111840
-
Schwarz, W. and I. M. Kens, 2004. Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with mannual and saccadic responses. Percept. Psychophys., 66(4): 651-664.
CrossRef PMid:15311664
-
Shaki, S. and W.M. Petrusic, 2005. On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effect with comparative judgments. Psychonomic Bull. Rev., 12(5): 931-937.
CrossRef PMid:16524013
-
Shaki, S., M.H. Fischer and W.H. Petrusic, 2009. Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychon. B. Rev., 16(2): 328-331.
CrossRef PMid:19293102
-
Shen, M.W., Y. Tian and H.J. Ding, 2006. The spatial representation of one-digit Arabic numbers. Chin. Psycholog. Sci., 29(2): 258-262.
-
Tan, S. and P. Dixon, 2011. Repetition and the SNARC effect with one-and two-digit numbers. Can. J. Exp. Psychol., 65(2): 84-97.
CrossRef PMid:21668090
-
Yang, J.Q., 2009. The different learning degree of SNARC effect on hearing Greek letter. J. Liaoning Educ. Admin. Inst., 26(11): 82-84.
-
Zebian, S., 2005. Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the REVERS SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates and illiterate Arabic speakers. J. Cogn. Cult., 5(1-2): 165-190.
CrossRef
-
Zhu, X.R. and C. Liu, 2008. Conflict adaptation under the SNARC effect: An ERP sudy. Acta Psycholog. Sinica, 40(3): 283-290.
CrossRef
Competing interests
The authors have no competing interests.
Open Access Policy
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Copyright
The authors have no competing interests.
|
|
|
ISSN (Online): 2040-7467
ISSN (Print): 2040-7459 |
|
Information |
|
|
|
Sales & Services |
|
|
|